Sentence Processing
Keywords: incremental parsing, expectations, psychological evidence, cognitive parsing, optimal parsing, cognitive models for
sentence processing, psycholinguistics, garden path
See also: OptimalityTheory,
IncrementalComputation,
CognitiveNeuroscience,
SentenceProcessingReading
Sources:
-
[BaderEtal04]
-
Markus Bader, Jana Häussler, and Josef Bayer.
Toward an integrated model of structure and frequency.
In Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing, AMLaP.
Laboratoire Parole et Langage, CNRS, Université de Provence, September 2004.
-
[BudiuAnderson04]
-
Raluca Budiu and John R. Anderson.
Interpretation-based processing: a unified theory of semantic
sentence comprehension.
In Cognitive Science [Budiu01], pages 1--44.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: We present interpretation-based processing
--- a theory of sentence processing that builds a syntactic
and a semantic representation for a sentence and assigns an
interpretation to the sentence as soon as possible. That
interpretation can further participate in comprehension and
in lexical processing and is vital for relating the sentence
to the prior discourse. Our theory offers a unified account
of the processing of literal sentences, metaphoric sentences,
and sentences containing semantic illusions. It also explains
how text can prime lexical access. We show that word
literality is a matter of degree and that the speed and
quality of comprehension depend both on how similar words are
to their antecedents in the preceding text and how salient
the sentence is with respect to the preceding text.
Interpretation-based processing also reconciles superficially
contradictory findings about the difference in processing
times for metaphors and literals. The theory has been
implemented in ACT-R (Anderson & Lebiere, 1998)
-
[KempenHarbusch03]
-
Gerard Kempen and Karin Harbusch.
An artificial opposition between grammaticality and frequency:
comment on [bornkessel, schlesewsky, and friederici (2002)].
In Cognition [BornkesselEtal02], pages 205--210.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: In a recent Cognition paper (Cognition 85
(2002) B21), Bornkessel, Schlesewsky, and Friederici report
ERP data that they claim "show that online processing
difficulties induced by word order variations in German
cannot be attributed to the relative infrequency of the
constructions in question, but rather appear to reflect the
application of grammatical principles during parsing" (p.
B21). In this commentary we demonstrate that the posited
contrast between grammatical principles and construction
(in)frequency as sources of parsing problems is artificial
because it is based on factually incorrect assumptions about
the grammar of German and on inaccurate corpus frequency data
concerning the German constructions involved.
-
[Sturt03]
-
Patrick Sturt.
The time-course of the application of binding constraints in
reference resolution.
Journal of Memory and Language, 48(3):542--562, 2003.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: We report two experiments which examined
the role of binding theory in on-line sentence processing.
Participants' eye movements were recorded while they read
short texts which included anaphoric references with
reflexive anaphors (himself or herself). In each of the
experiments, two characters were introduced into the
discourse before the anaphor, and only one of these
characters was a grammatical antecedent for the anaphor in
terms of binding theory. Both experiments showed that
Principle A of the binding theory operates at the very
earliest stages of processing; early eyemovement measures
showed evidence of processing diffculty when the gender of
the reflexive anaphor mismatched the stereotypical gender of
the grammatical antecedent. However, the gender of the
ungrammatical antecedent had no effect on early processing,
although it affected processing during later stages in
Experiment 1. An additional experiment showed that the gender
of the ungrammatical antecedent also affected the likelihood
of participants settling on an ungrammatical final
interpretation. The results are interpreted in relation to
the notions of bonding and resolution in reference
processing.
-
[McKoonRatcliff03]
-
Gail McKoon and Roger Ratcliff.
Meaning through syntax: Language comprehension and the reduced
relative clause construction.
Psychological Review, 110(3):490--525, 2003.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: A new explanation is proposed for a long
standing question in psycholinguistics: Why are some reduced
relative clauses so difficult to comprehend? It is proposed
that the meanings of some verbs like race are incompatible
with the meaning of the reduced relative clause and that this
incompatibility makes sentences like The horse raced past the
barn fell unacceptable. In support of their hypotheses, the
authors show that reduced relatives of The horse raced past
the barn fell type occur in naturally produced sentences with
a near-zero probability, whereas reduced relatives with other
verbs occur with a probability of about 1 in 20. The authors
also support the hypotheses with a number of psycholinguistic
experiments and corpus studies.
-
[KamideEtal03]
-
Yuki Kamide, Gerry Altmann, and Sarah L. Haywood.
The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing:
Evidence from anticipatory eye movements.
Journal of Memory and Language, 49:133--156, 2003.
see Corrigendum.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: Three eye-tracking experiments using the
visual-world paradigm are described that explore the basis by
which thematic dependencies can be evaluated in advance of
linguistic input that unambiguously signals those
dependencies. Following Altmann and Kamide (1999), who found
that selectional information conveyed by a verb can be used
to anticipate an upcoming Theme, we attempt to draw here a
more precise picture of the basis for such anticipatory
processing. Our data from two studies in English and one in
Japanese suggest that (a) verb-based information is not
limited to anticipating the immediately following
(grammatical) object, but can also anticipate later occurring
objects (e.g., Goals), (b) in combination with information
conveyed by the verb, a pre-verbal argument (Agent) can
constrain the anticipation of a subsequent Theme, and (c) in
a head-final construction such as that typically found in
Japanese, both syntactic and semantic constraints extracted
from pre-verbal arguments can enable the anticipation, in
effect, of a further forthcoming argument in the absence of
their head (the verb). We suggest that such processing is the
hallmark of an incremental processor that is able to draw on
different sources of information (some non-linguistic) at the
earliest possible opportunity to establish the fullest
possible interpretation of the input at each moment in time.
-
[HopfEtal03]
-
Jens-Max Hopf, Markus Bader, Michael Meng, and Josef Bayer.
Is human sentence parsing serial or parallel? evidence from
event-related brain potentials.
Cognitive Brain Research, 15:165--177, 2003.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: In this ERP study we investigate the
processes that occur in syntactically ambiguous German
sentences at the point of disambiguation. Whereas most
psycholinguistic theories agree on the view that processing
difficulties arise when parsing preferences are disconfirmed
(so-called garden-path effects), important differences exist
with respect to theoretical assumptions about the parser s
recovery from a misparse. A key distinction can be made
between parsers that compute all alternative syntactic
structures in parallel (parallel parsers) and parsers that
compute only a single preferred analysis (serial parsers). To
distinguish empirically between parallel and serial parsing
models, we compare ERP responses to garden-path sentences
with ERP responses to truly ungrammatical sentences.
Garden-path sentences contain a temporary and ultimately
curable ungrammaticality, whereas truly ungrammatical
sentences remain so permanently a difference which gives rise
to different predictions in the two classes of parsing
architectures. At the disambiguating word, ERPs in both
sentence types show negative shifts of similar onset latency,
amplitude, and scalp distribution in an initial time window
between 300 and 500 ms. In a following time window (500 700
ms), the negative shift to garden-path sentences disappears
at right central parietal sites, while it continues in
permanently ungrammatical sentences. These data are taken as
evidence for a strictly serial parser. The absence of a
difference in the early time window indicates that temporary
and permanent ungrammaticalities trigger the same kind of
parsing responses. Later differences can be related to
successful reanalysis in garden-path but not in ungrammatical
sentences.
-
[Ferreira03]
-
Fernanda Ferreira.
The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences.
Cognitive Psychology, 47(2):164--203, 2003.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: Research on language comprehension has
focused on the resolution of syntactic ambiguities, and most
studies have employed garden-path sentences to determine the
system's preferences and to assess its use of nonsyntactic
sources information. A topic that has been neglected is how
syntactically challenging but essentially unambiguous
sentences are processed, including passives and object-clefts
sentences that require thematic roles to be assigned in an
atypical order. The three experiments described here tested
the idea that sentences are processed both algorithmically
and heuristically. Sentences were presented aurally and the
participants task was to identify the thematic roles in the
sentence (e.g., Who was the do-er?). The rst experiment
demonstrates that passives are frequently and systematically
misinterpreted, especially when they express implausible
ideas. The second shows that the surface frequency of a
syntactic form does not determine ease of processing, as
active sentences and subject-clefts were comprehended equally
easily despite the rareness of the latter type. The third
experiment compares the processing of subject- and
object-clefts, and the results show that they are similar to
actives and passives, respectively, again despite the
infrequent occurrence in English of any type of cleft. The
results of the three experiments suggest that a comprehensive
theory of language comprehension must assume that simple
processing heuristics are used during processing in addition
to (and perhaps sometimes instead of) syntactic algorithms.
Moreover, the experiments support the idea that language
processing is often based on shallow processing, yielding a
merely good enough rather than a detailed linguistic
representation of an utterance s meaning.
-
[BornkesselEtal02]
-
Ina Bornkessel, Matthias Schlesewsky, and Angela D. Friederici.
Grammar overrides frequency: evidence from the online processing of
flexible word order.
Cognition, 85(2):B21--B30, September 2002.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: We show that online processing difficulties
induced by word order variations in German cannot be
attributed to the relative infrequency of the constructions
in question, but rather appear to reflect the application of
grammatical principles during parsing. Event-related brain
potentials revealed that dative-marked objects in the initial
position of an embedded sentence do not elicit a
neurophysiologically distinct response from subjects, whereas
accusative-marked objects do. These differences are
predictable on the basis of grammatical distinctions (i.e.
underlying linguistic properties), but not on the basis of
frequency information (i.e. a superficial linguistic
property). We therefore conclude that the former, but not the
latter, guides syntactic integration during online parsing.
-
[Crocker02]
-
Matthew W. Crocker.
Review of ”sentence comprehension: The integration of habits and
rules”.
In Computational Linguistics [TownsendBever01], pages
238 -- 241.
-
[TraxlerEtal02]
-
Matthew J. Traxler, Robin K. Morris, and Rachel E. Seely.
Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from
eye-movements.
Journal of Memory and Language, 47:69--90, 2002.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: Three eye-movement-monitoring experiments
investigated processing of sentences containing
subject-relative and object-relative clauses. The first
experiment showed that sentences containing object-relative
clauses were more difficult to process than sentences
containing subject-relative clauses during the relative
clause and the matrix verb. The second experiment manipulated
the plausibility of the sentential subject and the noun
within the relative clause as the agent of the action
represented by the verb in the relative clause. Readers
experienced greater difficulty during processing of sentences
containing object-relative clauses than subject-relative
clauses. The third experiment manipulated the animacy of the
sentential subject and the noun within the relative clause.
This experiment demonstrated that the difficulty associated
with object-relative clauses was greatly reduced when the
sentential subject was inanimate. We interpret the results
with respect to theories of syntactic parsing.
-
[SpiveyEtal02]
-
Michael J. Spivey, Michael K. Tanenhaus, Kathleen M. Eberhard, and Julie C.
Sedivy.
Eye movements and spoken language comprehension: Effects of visual
context on syntactic ambiguity resolution.
Cognitive Psychology, 45(4):447--481, 2002.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: When participants follow spoken
instructions to pick up and move objects in a visual
workspace, their eye movements to the objects are closely
time-locked to referential expressions in the instructions.
Two experiments used this methodology to investigate the
processing of the temporary ambiguities that arise because
spoken language unfolds over time. Experiment 1 examined the
processing of sentences with a temporarily ambiguous
prepositional phrase (e.g., Put the apple on the towel in the
box) using visual contexts that supported either the normally
preferred initial interpretation (the apple should be put on
the towel) or the less-preferred interpretation (the apple is
already on the towel and should be put in the box). Eye
movement patterns clearly established that the initial
interpretation of the ambiguous phrase was the one consistent
with the context. Experiment 2 replicated these results using
prerecorded digitized speech to eliminate any possibility of
prosodic differences across conditions or experimenter
demand. Overall, the ndings are consistent with a broad
theoretical framework in which real-time language
comprehension immediately takes into account a rich array of
relevant nonlinguistic context.
-
[GrodnerEtal02]
-
Daniel Grodner, Edward Gibson, and Susanne Tunstall.
Syntactic complexity in ambiguity resolution.
Journal of Memory and Language, 46(2):267--295, 2002.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: This article presents two self-paced
reading experiments which investigate the role of storage
costs associated with maintaining incomplete syntactic
dependencies in structural ambiguity resolution. We argue
that previous work has been equivocal regarding syntactic
influences because it has examined ambiguities where there is
little or no resource differential between competing
alternatives. The candidate structures of the ambiguities
explored here incur substantially different storage costs.
The results indicate that storage-based biases can be
sufficiently powerful to create difficulty for a structural
alternative even when it is promoted by nonsyntactic factors.
These findings are incorporated into a model of ambiguity
resolution in which structural biases operate as independent
graded constraints in selecting between structural
alternatives.
-
[FilipEtal02]
-
Hana Filip, Michael K. Tanenhaus, Gregory N. Carlson, Paul D. Allopenna, and
Joshua Blatt.
Reduced Relatives Judged Hard Require Constraint-Based
Analyses, pages 255--280.
Sentence Processing and the lexicon: formal, computational, and
experimental perspectives. Benjamins, Amsterdam, 2002.
-
[TownsendBever01]
-
David J. Townsend and Thomas G. Bever.
Sentence comprehension: The integration of habits and rules.
Language, speech, and communication series. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA,
May 2001.
[ http ]
-
[KellerAsudeh01]
-
Frank Keller and Ash Asudeh.
Constraints on linguistic coreference: Structural vs. pragmatic
factors.
In Johanna D. Moore and Keith Stenning, editors, Proceedings of
the 23rd. Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pages
483--488, Mahwah, NJ, 2001. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: Binding theory is the component of grammar
that regulates the interpretation of noun phrases. Certain
syntactic configurations involving picture noun phrases
(PNPs) are problematic for the standard formulation of
binding theory, which has prompted competing proposals for
revisions of the theory. Some authors have proposed an
account based on structural constraints, while others have
argued that anaphors in PNPs are exempt from binding theory,
but subject to pragmatic restrictions. In this paper, we
present an experimental study that aims to resolve this
dispute. The results show that structural factors govern the
binding possibilities in PNPs, while pragmatic factors play
only a limited role. However, the structural factors
identified differ from the ones standardly assumed.
-
[FriedericiEtal01]
-
A. D. Friederici, A. Mecklinger, K. Spencer, K. Steinhauer, and E. Donchin.
Syntactic parsing preferences and their on-line revisions: A
spatio-temporal analysis of event-related brain potentials.
Cognitive Brain Research, 11:305--323, 2001.
[ .pdf ]
-
[ChristiansonEtal01]
-
Kiel Christianson, Andrew Hollingworth, John F. Halliwell, and Fernanda
Ferreira.
Thematic roles assigned along the garden path linger.
Cognitive Psychology, 42:368--407, 2001.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: In the literature dealing with the
reanalysis of garden path sentences such as While the
man hunted the deer ran into the woods, it is generally
assumed either that people completely repair their initial
incorrect syntactic representations to yield a final
interpretation whose syntactic structure is fully consistent
with the input string or that the parse fails. In a series of
five experiments, we explored the possibility that partial
reanalyses take place. Specifically, we examined the
conditions under which part of the initial incorrect analysis
persists at the same time that part of the correct final
analysis is constructed. In Experiments 1a and 1b, we found
that both the length of the ambiguous region and the
plausibility of the ultimate interpretation affected the
likelihood that such sentences would be fully reanalyzed. In
Experiment 2, we compared garden path sentences with
non-garden path sentences and compared performance on two
different types of comprehension questions. In Experiments 3a
and 3b, we constructed garden path sentences using a small
class of syntactically unique verbs to provide converging
evidence against the position that people employ some sort of
”general reasoning” or pragmatic inference when faced with
syntactically difficult garden paths. The results from these
experiments indicate that reanalysis of such sentences is not
always complete, so that comprehenders often derive an
interpretation for the full sentence in which part of the
initial misanalysis persists. We conclude that the goal of
language processing is not always to create an idealized
structure, but rather to create a representation that is good
enough to satisfy the comprehender that an appropriate
interpretation has been obtained.
-
[CarlsonEtal01]
-
Katy Carlson, Jr. Charles Clifton, and Lyn Frazier.
Prosodic boundaries in adjunct attachment.
Journal of Memory and Language, 45(1):58--81, 2001.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: Five studies explored the processing of
ambiguous sentences like ”Martin maintained that the CEO
lied when the investigation started/at the start of the
investigation”. The central question was why particular
prosodic boundaries have the effects they do. A written
questionnaire provided baseline preferences and suggested
that clausal adjuncts (”when the investigation started”)
receive more high attachments than nonclausal adjuncts (”at
the start of the investigation”). Four auditory studies
manipulated the prosodic boundary before the adjunct clause
and the prosodic boundary between the matrix clause and its
complement. They disconfirm every version of an account where
only the local boundary before the adjunct is important,
whether the account is based on the acoustic magnitude of the
boundary or its phonological type (an intermediate boundary
characterized by the presence of a phrase accent vs. an
intonational phrase boundary characterized by both a phrase
accent and a boundary tone). Instead the results support use
of the global prosodic context, especially the relative size
of the local boundary and the distant boundary.
-
[Budiu01]
-
Raluca Budiu.
The Role of Background Knowledge in Sentence Processing.
Doctoral dissertation, School of Computer Science, Carnegie Mellon
University, 2001.
[ .pdf ]
In this dissertation I describe a cognitive model of
sentence processing. The model operates at the semantic level
and can apply to verification or comprehension of metaphoric
or literal sentences, isolated or embedded in discourse. It
uses an incremental search--and--match mechanism to find a
long-term--memory referent (interpretation) for an input
sentence. The search is guided by cues such as the last few
words read or previous tentative interpretations. The process
of comprehension produces a propositional representation for
the input sentence and also keeps track of local
comprehension failures.
The model is implemented in the ACT-R framework and offers a
scalable solution to the problem of language comprehension:
its performance (in terms of speed and accuracy) is roughly
invariant to the number of facts held in the long-term
memory. Its predictions match data from psycholinguistic
studies with human subjects. Specifically, the
sentence-processing model can simulate the comprehension and
verification of metaphoric and literal sentences,
metaphor-position effects on sentence comprehension, semantic
illusions and their dependence on semantic similarity between
the distortion and the undistorted term. The products of the
sentence-processing model can explain the pattern of sentence
recall in text-memory experiments.
This dissertation also explores the modeling alternatives
faced by the design of a sentence-processing model. I show
that, to achieve comprehension speed comparable to that of
humans, a model must minimize the explicit search process and
rely on semantic associations among words. I also investigate
how the representation chosen for propositions and meanings
affects the comprehension process in a production-system
framework such as ACT-R.
-
[AlmorEtal01]
-
Amit Almor, Maryellen C. MacDonald, Daniel Kempler, Elaine S. Andersen, and
Lorraine K. Tyler.
Comprehension of long distance number agreement in probable
alzheimer's disease.
Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(1), 2001.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: Two cross-modal naming experiments examined
the role of working memory in processing sentences and
discourses of various lengths. In Experiment 1, 10 memory
impaired patients with probable Alzheimer s disease (AD) and
-
-
-
-
-
- healthy elderly control participants showed similar sensitivity to violations of subject-verb number agreement in a short sentence condition and similar degradation to this sensitivity in a long sentence condition. Performance in neither length condition correlated with performance on working memory tasks, suggesting that the processes involved in interpreting a grammatical dependency between adjacent and nonadjacent elements are different from those required in the working memory tasks. In Experiment 2, the same 10 AD patients were less sensitive than the 10 control participants to pronounantecedent number agreement violations in a short discourse condition, but neither group was affected by additional length. In this experiment, performance in both the short and long conditions correlated with working memory performance. These results show that grammatical and discourse dependencies pose different memory and processing demands, and that these differences are not simply due to differences in the amount of intervening material between dependent words. The results also suggest that while the working memory de cits characteristic of AD do not interfere with on-line grammatical processing within sentences, they do compromise on-line discourse processing across sentences.
-
[CrockerBrants00]
-
Matthew Crocker and Thorsten Brants.
Wide-coverage probabilistic sentence processing.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(6):647--669, November
2000.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: This paper describes a fully implemented,
broad coverage model of human syntactic processing. The model
uses probabilistic parsing techniques which combine phrase
structure, lexical category, and limited subcategory
probabilities with an incremental, left-to-right the system
to achieve good accuracy on typical, "garden variety"
language (i.e. when tested on corpora). Furthermore, the
incremental probabilistic ranking of the preferred analyses
during parsing also naturally explains observed human
behaviour for a range of garden-path structures. We do not
make strong psychological claims about the specific
probabilistic mechanism discussed here, which is limited by a
number of practical considerations. Rather, we argue
incremental probabilistic parsing models are, in general,
extremely well suited to explaining this dual nature -
generally good and occasionally pathological - of human
linguistic performance.
-
[PearlmutterMendelson00]
-
Neal J. Pearlmutter and A. A. Mendelson.
Serial versus parallel sentence comprehension.
Manuscript in revision, August 31 2000.
[ .ps ]
-
[SchaferEtal00]
-
Amy J. Schafer, Shari R. Speer, Paul Warren, and S. David White.
Intonational disambiguation in sentence production and comprehension.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29:169--182, 2000.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: Speakers prosodic marking of syntactic
constituency is often measured in sentence reading tasks that
lack realistic situational constraints on speaking. Results
from such studies can be criticized because the pragmatic
goals of readers differ dramatically from those of speakers
in typical conversation. On the other hand, recordings of
unscripted speech do not readily yield the carefully
controlled contrasts required for many research purposes. Our
research employs a cooperative game task, in which two
speakers use utterances from a predetermined set to negotiate
moves around gameboards. Results from a set of early versus
late closure ambiguities suggest that speakers signal this
syntactic difference with prosody even when the utterance
context fully disambiguates the structure. Phonetic and
phonological analyses show reliable prosodic disambiguation
in speakers productions; results of a comprehension task
indicate that listeners can successfully use prosodic cues to
categorize syntactically ambiguous fragments as portions of
early or late closure utterances.
-
[PickeringEtal00]
-
Martin J. Pickering, Matthew J. Traxler, and Matthew W. Crocker.
Ambiguity resolution in sentence processing: Evidence against
frequency-based accounts.
Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3):447--475, 2000.
[ .pdf ]
Abstract: Three eye-tracking experiments investigated
two frequency-based processing accounts: the serial
lexical-guidance account, in which people adopt the analysis
compatible with the most likely subcategorization of a verb;
and the serial-likelihood account, in which people adopt the
analysis that they would regard as the most likely analysis,
given the information available at the point of ambiguity.
The results demonstrate that neither of these accounts
explains readers performance. Instead people preferred to
attach noun phrases as arguments of verbs even when such
analyses were unlikely to be correct. We suggest that these
results fit well with a model in which the processor
initially favors informative analyses.
-
[BrantsCrocker00]
-
Thorsten Brants and Matthew Crocker.
Probabilistic parsing and psychological plausibility.
In Proceedings of the 18th International Conference on
Computational Linguistics, 2000.
[ .ps.gz ]
Abstract: Given the recent evidence for probabilistic
mechanisms in models of human ambiguity resolution, this
paper investigates the plausibility of exploiting current
wide-coverage, probabilistic parsing techniques to model
human linguistic performance. In particular, we investigate
the performance of standard stochastic parsers when they are
revised to operate incrementally, and with reduced memory
resources. We present techniques for ranking and filtering
analyses, together with experimental results. Our results
confirm that stochastic parsers which adhere to these
psychologically motivated constraints achieve good
performance. Memory can be reduced down to 1 exhausitve search) without reducing recall and precision.
Additionally, these models exhibit substantially faster
performance. Finally, we argue that this general result is
likely to hold for more sophisticated, and
psycholinguistically plausible, probabilistic parsing
models.
-
[GibsonPearlmutter98]
-
E. Gibson and Neal J. Pearlmutter.
Constraints on sentence comprehension.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2:262--268, May 15 1998.
[ .ps ]
-
[CristeaWebber97]
-
D. Cristea and Bonie Lynn Webber.
Expectations in incremental discourse processing.
In Proc. 35th Annual Meeting of the Association for
Comptutational Linguistics, Madrid, July 1997.
[ .html ]
-
[Phillips96]
-
Colin Phillips.
Order and Structure.
PhD thesis, MIT, September 1996.
[ .ps ]
The aim of this thesis is to argue for the following two
main points. First, that grammars of natural language
construct sentences in a strictly left-to-right fashion, i.e.
starting at the beginning of the sentence and ending at the
end. Second, that there is no distinction between the grammar
and the parser.
In the area of phrase structure, I show that the left-right
derivations forced by the principle Merge Right can account
for the apparent contradictions that different tests of
constituency show, and that they also provide an explanation
for why the different tests yield the results that they do.
Diagnostics discussed include coordination, movement,
ellipsis, binding, right node raising and scope.
I present a preliminary account of the interface of phonology
and morphology with syntax based on left-right derivations. I
show that this approach to morphosyntax allows for a uniform
account of locality in head movement and clitic placement,
explains certain directional asymmetries in phonology-syntax
mismatches and head movement, and allows for a tighter
connection between syntactic and phonological phrases than
commonly assumed.
In parsing I argue that a wide range of structural biases in
ambiguity resolution can be accounted for by the single
principle Branch Right, which favors building right-branching
structures wherever possible. Evidence from novel and
existing experimental work is presented which shows that
Branch Right has broader empirical coverage than other
proposed structural parsing principles. Moreover, Branch
Right is not a parsing-specific principle: it is
independently motivated as an economy principle of syntax in
the chapters on syntax.
The combination of these results from syntax and parsing
makes it possible to claim that the parser and the grammar
are identical. The possibility that the parser and the
grammar were identical or extremely similar was explored in
the early 1960s, but is widely considered to have been
discredited by the end of that decade. I show that arguments
against this model which were once valid no longer apply
given left-to-right syntax and the view of the parser
proposed here.
-
[MacDonald94]
-
Maryellen C. MacDonald.
Probabilistic constraints and syntactic ambiguity resolution.
Language and Cognitive processes, 9:157--201, 1994.
Signature R30239.
[ .pdf ]
-
[Lombardo92]
-
Vincenzo Lombardo.
Incremental dependency parsing.
In Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics,
pages 291--293, 1992.
[ .pdf ]
-
[PriceEtal91]
-
Patti Price, Mari Ostendorf, Stefanie Shattuck-Hufnagel, and C. Fong.
The use of prosody in syntactic disambiguation.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 90:2956--2970,
1991.
Abstract: Prosodic structure and syntactic structure
are not identical; neither are they unrelated. Knowing when
and how the two correspond could yield better quality speech
synthesis, could aid in the disambiguation of competing
syntactic hypotheses in speech understanding, and could lead
to a more comprehensive view of human speech processing. In a
set of experiments involving 35 pairs of phonetically similar
sentences representing seven types of structural contrasts,
the perceptual evidence shows that some, but not all, of the
pairs can be disambiguated on the basis of prosodic
differences. The phonological evidence relates the
disambiguation primarily to boundary phenomena, although
prominences sometimes play a role. Finally, phonetic analyses
describing the attributes of these phonological markers
indicate the importance of both absolute and relative
measures.
Paul Gorrell (1995): Syntax and parsing.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1995.
Book Review: (Martin Volk:
A review of "Syntax and Parsing" by Paul Gorrell. In: Literary and Linguistic Computing. Vol.11 (2). 1996. 104-105)
The title of this book is perhaps misleading. As a computational linguist I had expected the book to be about computational approaches to natural language parsing. But this book deals with cognitive parsing, i.e. modelling the parsing processes within the human brain. The syntax background is provided by Government Binding (GB) Theory.
Not being an expert in that area I decided to review the book from the perspective of an interested researcher from a neighboring field, trying to learn about GB and its role in human language processing and in cognitive linguistics.
Paul Gorrell outlines a model of human sentence processing from a GB perspective. In this model he distinguishes between primary and secondary operations. Primary operations include building up a phrase structure tree including dominance and precedence relations. These operations are deterministic, i.e. they are not redone because of ambiguities not even in the case of garden path phenomena. In contrast, secondary operations are not deterministic. These include government, c-command, theta assignment, case assignment, and binding. These operations serve to interpret the structure being established by the primary operations. When encountering material that requires reanalysis of interpretation the secondary operations are redone.
Gorrell takes great care to argue that this model is in accordance with empirical findings in cognitive linguistics experiments such as reaction time tests, eye movement tests, shadowing tests etc. Some of these tests he has performed himself, but mostly he tries to accommodate his proposal within the findings of many other researchers in the field.
The book is structured into 6 chapters. Chapter 1 `Introduction' sets the goals:
(p.1) It is a central thesis of this book that recent work within Government Binding (GB) theory (Chomsky 1981, and subsequent work) raises questions about the nature of syntactic knowledge that have long concerned researchers into syntactic processing (parsing).
(p.4) The aim of this book is to give as much attention to the form of the grammar as to the form of the parser.
Chapter 2 gives an introduction to GB theory, distinguishing between derivational (with levels: d-structure, s-structure, logical form) and representational (monostratal) GB. Gorrell assumes a monostratal version for his model. Chapter 3 is devoted to `Analyses of previous work'. It is a detailed survey of the research in human sentence parsing. It looks at the field from many different perspectives providing a host of examples showing that processing difficulties are not restricted to the garden path in
The horse raced past the barn fell.
Let me cite three more examples to reflect the flavor of the discussion:
(p.50) Without her contributions would be inadequate.
(p.53) We gave the man the grant proposal was written by last year a copy of this year's proposal.
(p.55) Though George kept on reading the story really bothered him.
Elaborating on chapter 3, chapter 4 is the heart of the book. Here, the author outlines his model, contrasting it to competing approaches and showing briefly how it applies to languages other than English, namely head-final languages like Japanese and German. Chapter 5 looks at how semantic and pragmatic factors influence structure building in the human brain. The chapter leaves me with the impression that this area is still little explored and that it is much harder to design good experiments. Nevertheless, I agree with Gorrell that issues such as absolute and relative frequency of word occurrence, as well as priming by context are keys to an improved understanding of the ease and speed of human sentence processing. Chapter 6 concludes the book with a short summary.
The book is a scientific work in the best tradition. It lays the groundwork, reports on experiments, presents a model incorporating the experimental results and defends the model against competing proposals. It comes with an extensive reference list. As such the book addresses mostly researchers in a relatively narrow scientific domain, namely people working in cognitive linguistics. I found it demanding to read even with a background in a neighboring discipline. The book is certainly not an introduction to the field nor can it be used as a textbook.
One reason for the efforts required from a thoughtful reader is the pervasive use of acronyms. Here are just two examples:
(p.51) The motivation for the PPP processing input in accord with the strategies LC and MA is that ...
(p.150) Before contrasting MT and IT and motivating a theory of SI parsing, it is necessary ...
Certainly, all but the most obvious acronyms are introduced in the book at one point or another. But they add up to such a number that they result in a cognitive burden. This is regrettable, especially since an acronym glossary is missing.
Other than this, the book has been crafted with great care. Layout and tree structure graphs are excellent and there are very few misspellings. One would wish that more publications were on such a level of formal quality.
Bradley L. Pritchett (1992): Grammatical Competence and Parsing Performance
Carnegie Mellon University.
Book Review: by Gerry Altmann, University of Sussex:
PDF
Markus Bader, Michael Meng (1999): Subject-Object Ambiguities in German Embedded Clauses: An Across-the-Board Comparison
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, Vol.28, No.2. (
PDF)
Abstract:
This paper examines the processing of embedded clauses in German which are ambiguous between a subject-before-object and an object-before-subject order. In an experiment using a speeded grammaticality judgment task, four types of locally ambiguous clauses were compared: (i) sentences involving movement of a definite noun phrase (NP), (ii) sentences involving pronoun movement, (iii) relative clauses, and (iv) embedded questions. We found that readers were consistently garden-pathed in the object-before-subject condition, regardless of sentence type. Furthermore, there were considerable differences with respect to garden-path strength. The garden-path effect was strongest for sentences involving scrambling. In addition, sentences involving pronoun movement induced more processing difficulty than embedded questions and relative clauses. We argue that our findings can be best explained within a serial processing model that acknowledges both syntactic and nonsyntactic influences on reanalysis and that can account for graded effects of garden-path strength.
Fabrizio Costa, P. Frasconi, V. Lombardo, and G. Soda (2003): Towards Incremental Parsing of Natural Language using Recursive Neural Networks
Applied Intelligence, 19 (1-2):9-25, July - October, 2003,
PDF
F. Costa, V. Lombardo, P. Frasconi and G. Soda: Wide coverage incremental parsing by learning attachment preferences
Conference of the Italian Association for Artificial Intelligence, 2001,
PDF
Gardent Claire and Bonnie Webber (1998): Describing Discourse Semantics.
Proceedings of the 4th TAG+ workshop, Philadelphia, US, 1998. (
PS,
citeseer)
(move this paper into another category)